Threaded index     Date index     FAQ


Young Children's Agency - J. Murray, 2025 [link]

Posted by Manstuprator on 2025-September-15 21:01:29, Monday
In reply to Children's agency--10-year-old's study 2019 [link] posted by Manstuprator on 2025-September-15 20:15:27, Monday

There ARE folks who understand that kids benefit greatly by being given agency. But those folks are not psychologists who are part of the Child Sexual Abuse industry. They are educators and genuine researchers.

The following is a recent editorial (2025) which discusses children and agency. Of course, sex is taboo, so no mention of children's sexual agency is included.

I do have materials on children's sexual agency, but I'd have to search for them.

NOTE: In the following, I didn't put italics where necessary on the references. My bad.

M.
Some day the taboo will lessen, or even disappear. Some day...

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF EARLY YEARS EDUCATION
2025, VOL. 33, NO. 2, 237–243
https://doi.org/10.1080/09669760.2025.2495432
Pages 237-243
Author: Jane Murray
Centre for Education and Research, University of Northampton, Northampton, UK
Jane.Murray@northampton.ac.uk http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7000-0901

EDITORIAL

Young children’s agency

Introduction

Recently, my grandson aged 15 months left a trail of devastation in the kitchen at his family
home. While my daughter-in-law, who is his mother, was busy on the other side of the
kitchen, he had opened the cupboard where the baking ingredients are stored. His mother
turned around to see tins and packets scattered all over the floor as well as a generous
layer of flour with little footprints in it. My grandson was heading out of the kitchen with
floury feet. When she asked him ‘Did you do that?’, he walked back to the flour, looked up
at her, then pointed to the mess, nodded and responded ‘Yes’. His mother then asked him:
‘Are you going to clean it up?’ and he squatted and began moving the flour about before
his mother handed him a little floor brush which he pushed around in a genuine effort to
clean. This quotidian event at home exemplifies the opportunities the youngest member of
our family has at home to learn through experience, play and exploration. It is also an
example of the many affordances he is offered that enable him to choose what to do, and
to act independently in line with what he decides. In short, it is an example of a little boy’s
agency at 15 months. In this paper I argue that young children’s agency is important for
their development and learning. I begin by exploring definitions of agency and children’s
agency. I then consider why young children’s agency matters, before troubling some pro-
blems associated with young children’s agency and proposing potential ways forward in rei-
fying young children’s agency.

What is agency?

Giddens (1984) theorises agency as one element of structuration, in which he sees social
relations positioned ‘across time and space in virtue of the duality of structure’ (176). He
posits that structure in the form of society’s institutions and social practices influences indi-
viduals, whilst individuals can choose to act as active, independent agents in ways that can
change structure. In this way, society’s structure and individuals’ agency operate in symbiosis
(Giddens 1984).

There is some consensus in the literature regarding how agency is defined. A core
definition that is used commonly is ‘capacity to act’ (Emirbayer and Mische 1998, 999;
James and James 2008, 9; Manyukhina and Wyse 2019, 223; Schatzki 2025, 1; Thomson
and Maloy 2022, 27), though each source varies the definition slightly. Manyukhina and
Wyse (2019, 223) propose that agency is the ‘capacity to act independently’ while James
and James (2008, 9) define it as ‘the capacity of individuals to act independently’.
Thomson and Maloy (2022, 27) suggest that agency is ‘the capacity to act, and to make
choices and decisions about matters that concern you’, and Schatzki (2025, 1) contends
that ‘Agency is the capacity to act, alternatively, the exercise of this capacity. It is both a
power and an actuality’. For Killeen, Tillery, and Cabrera (2024, 2), agency is ‘the power of
choosing an action and effecting it. Sewell (1992, 2) sees agency as ‘the efficacy of human
action’, while Emirbayer and Mische (1998, 999) align with structuration theory in their
definition of agency as ‘a capacity to act rightly and effectively within particular concrete
life circumstances’. Giddens himself (1984, 14) argues that agency requires the capability
and ability ‘to deploy (chronically, in the flow of daily life) a range of causal powers, including
that of influencing those deployed by others’.

The synthesis of definitions across the range of literature suggests that agency may be
defined as an individual’s capacity, capability, ability, or power to make decisions, choose,
influence and effect actions independently.

Although agency is often used interchangeably with the terms autonomy and self-determi-
nation, but these three terms have different meanings (Sutterlüty and Tisdall 2019). Whereas
agency is widely defined as the capacity to act as outlined above, self-determination is seen as
both the option to choose as a human characteristic and society’s affordances to make our
own choices (Farini and Scollan 2025). On the other hand, autonomy is recognised as ‘an
inner endorsement of one’s actions, the sense that they emanate from oneself and are
one’s own’ (Deci and Ryan 1987, 1025).

What is children’s agency?

OHCHR (2006) recognises that children are social actors from birth, and the idea of chil-
dren’s agency has been leveraged powerfully within the field of ‘new’ sociology during the
early part of the twenty-first century (James, Jenks, and Prout 1998; Jenks 2005; Moss and
Petrie 2002). James, Jenks, and Prout (1998, 6) argue that this phenomenon has led to ‘the
recognition of children as particular persons’. Nevertheless, whilst definitions of children’s
agency have much in common with definitions of agency per se, the literature reveals
nuanced differences. Oswell (2013, 42) posits that children’s agency is ‘the capacity to do
things in the world, where that doing may be physical, cognitive, emotional or other, but
such that “being able to do” implies that children are not passive “blank slates”’. This view
suggests that children’s agency has implications for their education, as Manyukhina and
Wyse (2019) observe. OECD (2019, 32) recognises that in school, ‘students have the ability
and the will to positively influence their own lives and the world around them’ and defines
student agency as the ‘capacity to set a goal, reflect and act responsibly to effect change. It
is about acting rather than being acted upon; shaping rather than being shaped; and
making responsible decisions and choices rather than accepting those determined by
others’ (OECD 2019, 32).

Equally, in considering children’s agency in relation to curriculum, Manyukhina (2022,
506) resonates with Giddens’ structuration theory (1984) by proposing that ‘the enactment
of agency’ is contingent on both ‘appropriate structural opportunities’ and ‘individuals
who believe themselves capable of acting upon them’. Similarly to agency per se, children’s
agency is enacted in social contexts (Baker and Le Courtois 2022; Gallagher 2019; Sutterlüty
and Tisdall 2019). Wyness (2014, 13) posits that ‘Children as agents are immersed within the
social world and thus embedded in relations within which they have a formative influence.
The child agent is not only capable but also fully social’. Young children’s presentations of
agency in early childhood education (ECE) settings happen when the children have ‘oppor-
tunities to influence and make choices about how and what they learn in order to demonstrate
and expand their capabilities’ (Colegrove et al. 2021). For children’s agency to present and
flourish, learning environments must, then, offer affordances that promote children’s
agency. Such affordances give the agentic learner power and control to choose to engage
or not, and to direct their own actions and thoughts (Baker, Le Courtois, and Eberhart
2021). However, this does not always happen.

Adults tend to retain power in their interactions with children, especially younger chil-
dren. Although children sometimes attempt to resist adults’ power by asserting their own
agency, for example by non-compliance or having a tantrum, it is more usual for children
to ‘meekly conform’ to avoid undesirable consequences (Jerome and Starkey 2022, 440).
When adults fail to share power with children, children are ‘silenced … controlled, oppressed,
labelled and limited’ (Cannella 2002, 162); indeed, Fleet and Britt (2011, 143) observe that
‘children are often the most silenced participants in the educative process’. Furthermore,
younger children are unlikely to be afforded opportunities to make decisions about matters
that affect them, including their education (Lundy et al. 2024).

Additionally, OECD (2019, 34) proposes that children’s agency in educational settings
‘requires the ability to frame a guiding purpose and identify actions to achieve a goal’,
suggesting that unlike adults, children are not afforded the option to exercise agency
without a goal that is likely to have been decided by adults (Lundy et al. 2024). Despite
this disparity, even the youngest children are capable of agency driven by their own intention-
ality. Delafield-Butt and Trevarthen (2020, 1) argue that intentionality presents even before
birth in the unborn child’s movements which reveal their ‘awareness and a curious interest
in the world’ which continue postnatally in response to their environment, indicating
agentic neonates, infants and very young children.

As I have indicated, above, there are similarities between agency per se and children’s
agency but there are also differences. Children’s agency may by characterised as an individual
child’s capacity, capability and ability to choose, influence and effect actions, but it is unlikely
to include the power, independence and decision-making that are features of an adult’s
agency. Children are only free to enact their agency if the social context they are in
permits them so to do, and adults tend only to afford children agency if doing so suits the
adult agenda.

Why does young children’s agency matter?

Interest in children’s agency increased exponentially during the first decades of the twenty-
first century (Sutterlüty and Tisdall 2019). It is crucial that young children experience
agency for different reasons. Of particular importance for education are the associations of
young children’s agency with self-regulation and motivation, both of which promote
young children’s learning and academic success (Baker, Le Courtois, and Eberhart 2021;
Wang and Barrett 2013). Self-regulation supports executive functioning including working
memory, and controls feelings, thoughts, actions and attention (Hautakangas, Kumpulainen,
and Uusitalo 2021), while motivation – particularly intrinsic motivation – promotes learners’
mastery, engagement and enjoyment in learning, typically in activities based on curiosity,
exploration and play (Ryan and Deci 2020).

Equally, sense of agency and the intentionality that accompanies it constitute senses of self,
or ‘how we experience ourselves in relation to others’ (Stern 1985, 6). Senses of self ‘provides a
basic organising perspective for all interpersonal events’ from infancy (Klein 2012; Stern 1985,
6). Strong sense of self is associated with positive relationships, increased autonomy and posi-
tive self-esteem, whereas a weak sense of self can predict poor mental health (Flury and Ickes
2007; Hanley and Garland 2017).

Moreover, children’s agency is related to children’s rights (Jerome and Starkey 2022). In
part, this is because children’s agency and their voices are interlinked (Maybin 2013;
Spiteri 2024). Children’s voices are not only their verbal contributions in social contexts
but are also ‘views of children that are actively heard and valued as substantive contributions
to decisions affecting the children’s lives’ (Brooks and Murray 2016). Children’s agency
underpins and is promoted by Article 12 in the United Nations Convention on the Rights of
the Child (OHCHR 1989). Article 12 affords children who are ‘capable of forming their own
views’ the right ‘to express those views freely in all matters affecting the child, the views of the
child being given due weight in accordance with the age and maturity of the child’. Lundy
et al. (2024) refute that ‘age and maturity’ limits the affordance of due weight to
older children, arguing that the right of children to have their views ‘given due weight’
refers to every child who is ‘capable of forming their own views’. Given that neonates’ abilities
to assert their identities, present with ‘abstract structured knowledge’ and demonstrate inten-
tionality (Alderson, Hawthorne, and Killen 2008, 29; Delafield-Butt and Trevarthen 2020;
Gopnik 2011, 129–130), the youngest children are ‘capable of forming a view’, and by
doing so demonstrate ‘capacity to act’. Therefore, even the youngest children should be
afforded the right to the views they form being ‘given due weight’. Young children’s right
to express their views and have them taken seriously chimes with key features of agency;
young children have ‘capacities for informed decision-making’ and young children’s develop-
ment is enhanced when adults share power with them, for example by attuning to their modes
of communication (Lansdown and Karkara 2006).

Some problems with young children’s agency

Although there are good reasons why young children’s agency matters, political, cultural,
economic and social structures shape and limit young children’s agency (Thomson and
Maloy 2022). James, Jenks, and Prout (1998, 69) question whether affording children
agency might have a perverse effect, by inhibiting childhood freedoms. However, as
Thomson and Maloy (2022, 27) observe, ‘curriculum and pedagogical practices (both)
limit and enable what students can do’, so if we do not afford young children their right to
be heard when they express their views about their education, we also deny young children
the agency that could illuminate their rich capacity to form their own views on matters
that affect their lives (Lundy et al. 2024). Furthermore, early childhood policymakers tend
to focus on young children’s futures and what they may become, for example, orienting
early childhood development, care and pre-primary education towards school readiness
(United Nations 2024, 4.2). Yet young children generally operate in the ‘here and now’
(Graue and Walsh 1995), so they direct their agency to their present concerns. There exist,
therefore, tensions for educators and parents between accommodating young children’s
agency, whilst also trying to adhere to policymakers’ requirements.

How can we reify young children’s agency?

If we believe that young children should have opportunities to be agentic, there are some
directions of travel that we can take to improve matters. As a first step, policymakers, educa-
tors and parents must conceptualise children ‘as capable thinkers and doers’ (Adair 2014,
236). Part of this conceptualisation would be attending more effectively to young children’s
concerns by seeing young children ‘as human beings, not only “human becomings”’
(Qvortrup 1994, 18). Equally, if we accept that children’s agency is bound inextricably into
social structuration (Giddens 1984), we must also work harder to provide structures that
support children’s agency. For example, in order to promote children’s agency, educators
need sufficient agency of their own to co-create curricula and pedagogies with children
that afford the time, space, choices, opportunities for creativity that young children need
to flourish (Adair 2014; Colegrove et al. 2021). Enacting young children’s agency in ECE set-
tings requires greater equalisation of power relationships between adults and children, as well
as educators who attend to children’s voices in ECE settings, and pedagogies that foster young
children’s contributions and participation (Sirkko, Kyrönlampi, and Puroila 2019). Further-
more, policymakers, educators and parents could focus more on turning participation rights
policy into action, especially Article 12. As Lundy et al. (2024, 11) assert, ‘we should seek
young children’s perspectives on the quality of ECE wherever decisions are being made
that affect them’ whilst also acknowledging and explaining that it may not always be possible
or desirable to pursue every suggestion made by every child.

In summary

In this paper, I have explored definitions of agency and children’s agency, and considered why
young children’s agency matters. I have troubled some problems associated with young chil-
dren’s agency and I have drawn on a range of sources to indicate potential ways to reify young
children’s agency at policy and practice levels. Children’s agency is foundational for children’s
development, learning, education and lives from their earliest years (Adair 2014; OECD 2019;
Manyukhina and Wyse 2021). To this end, the agency that my 15-month-old grandson is for-
tunate to be afforded in his family home augurs well for his future.

Introduction to articles in this issue

The articles in this issue address four aspects of practice that address the topic of young chil-
dren’s agency in different ways: early educators’ autonomy and agency, young children’s
social and emotional learning, young children as thinkers and early educators’ professional
development. The first article in the issue focuses on ‘Educators’ risk-taking in high quality
early childhood education’ and is authored by Mandy Cooke, Frances Press and Sandie
Wong. Katariina, Waltzer, Sirpa Kärkkäinen and Sari Havu-Nuutinen then report research
focused on ‘Early childhood professionals’ pedagogical decision making’. Next, Lynn Ang, Han-
nelie du Preez, Anna-Barbara Du Plessis, Liz-Marie Basson, Liesel Ebersöhn, and Qing Gu
present their article on ‘The role of ECDE in supporting learning and well-being in rural
early childhood and primary schools in South Africa’. Veronica Mctaggart, Simon Stephens
and Rosemary McGill contribute an article entitled ‘An examination of the development of
children’s social and emotional competencies in pre-school’. Next, this issue features four
articles about young children as thinkers: ‘Using Philosophy for Children to Introduce the
Living/Non-living Distinction in Kindergarten’ by Renia Gasparatou, Marida Ergazaki and
Nikolitsa Kosmopoulou, ‘Classroom interaction and metacognition by “enculturation” of
thinking in early education’ by Isabel María Gómez-Barreto, Sonia Lara and Ruth Pinedo-
González, ‘“Young children nowadays are very smart in ICT”: Preschool teachers’ perceptions
of ICT Use’ by Chuanmei Dong, and ‘A Comparative Analysis of Creativity: Australian and
Iranian Early Childhood National Policy Frameworks’ by Somayeh Ba Akhlagh and Nicole
Leggett. Seongmi Lim has contributed ‘Preservice Teachers’ Perspectives of Play in Early Child-
hood Education’. Jessie Ming Sin Wong, Venus Sin Yee Tong, Selina Chan and Man Chun
contribute an article with the title ‘Beyond Constraints: Interpreting Voices to Reframe Prep-
aration of Teachers for Inclusive Kindergarten Classrooms’. Xinyue Yang, Rahman Bin Abdul,
Nazri Mohd and Yansong Sun have authored ‘The impact of teachers’ qualifications on early
childhood children’s development outcomes: A systematic literature review’. An article entitled
‘Quality Early Childhood Education and Care practices: skills, knowledge, and the graduate
competencies’ comes from Eva Mikuska, Nikki Fairchild, Alex Sabine, and Sarah Barton com-
pletes the collection of articles for this issue of International Journal of Early Years Education.

References

Adair, J. K. 2014. “Agency and Expanding Capabilities in Early Grade Classrooms: What it Could Mean for
Young Children.” Harvard Educational Review 84 (2): 217–241. https://doi.org/10.17763/haer.84.2.
y46vh546h41l2144.

Alderson, P., J. Hawthorne, and M. Killen. 2008. “The Participation Rights of Premature Babies.” In Beyond
Article 12: Essential Readings on Children’s Participation, edited by H. Van Beers, A. Invernizzi, and B.
Milne, 57–65. Bangkok: Black and White Publications.

Baker, S., and S. Le Courtois. 2022. “Agency, Children’s Voice and Adults’ Responsibility.” Education 3-13 50
(4): 435–438. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004279.2022.2052234.

Baker, S. T., S. Le Courtois, and J. Eberhart. 2021. “Making Space for Children’s Agency with Playful
Learning.” International Journal of Early Years Education 31 (2): 372–384. https://doi.org/10.1080/
09669760.2021.1997726.

Brooks, E., and J. Murray. 2016. “Ready, Steady, Learn: School Readiness and Children’s Voices in English
Early Childhood Settings.” Education 3-13 46 (2): 143–156. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004279.2016.
1204335.

Cannella, G. S. 2002. Deconstructing Early Childhood Education. New York: Peter Lang.

Colegrove, K. S.-S., M. E. McManus, J. K. Adair, and K. A. Payne. 2021. “Los Policas! Latinx Children’s Agency
in Highly Regulated Early Childhood Contexts.” Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood 24 (4): 453–467.
https://doi.org/10.1177/14639491211021595.

Deci, E. L., and R. M. Ryan. 1987. “The Support of Autonomy and the Control of Behavior.” Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 53 (6): 1024–1037. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.6.1024.

Delafield-Butt, J., and C. Trevarthen. 2020. “Infant Intentionality: Learning with Others.” In Encyclopedia of
Teacher Education, edited by M. Peters. Singapore: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-13-1179-6_
74-1.

Emirbayer, M., and A. Mische. 1998. “What Is Agency?” American Journal of Sociology 103 (4): 962–1023.
https://doi.org/10.1086/231294. https://www.jstor.org/stable/.

Farini, F., and A. Scollan. 2025. “Solving the Paradox: The Social Semantics of Children’s Right of Self-
Determination.” Socium 2 (1): 5–23. https://doi.org/10.62476/soc.2105.

Fleet, A., and C. Britt. 2011. “Seeing Spaces, Inhabiting Places.” In Researching Young Children’s Perspectives,
edited by D. Harcourt, B. Perry, and T. Waller, 142–162. London: Routledge.

Flury, J. M., and W. Ickes. 2007. “Having a Weak Versus Strong Sense of Self: The Sense of Self Scale (SOSS).”
Self and Identity 6 (4): 281–303. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298860601033208.

Gallagher, M. 2019. “Rethinking Children’s Agency: Power, Assemblages, Freedom and Materiality.” Global
Studies of Childhood 9 (3): 188–199. https://doi.org/10.1177/2043610619860993.

Giddens, A. 1984. The Constitution of Society. Cambridge: Polity Press.

Gopnik, A. 2011. “A Unified Account of Abstract Structure and Conceptual Change: Probabilistic Models and
Early Learning Mechanisms.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34 (3): 129–130. doi:10.1017/
S0140525X10002438.

Graue, M. E., and D. J. Walsh. 1995. “Children in Context: Interpreting the Here and Now of Children’s
Lives.” In Qualitative Research in Early Childhood Settings, edited by J. A. Hatch, 135–154. Westport,
CT: Praeger.

Hanley, A. W., and E. L. Garland. 2017. “Clarity of Mind: Structural Equation Modeling of Associations
Between Dispositional Mindfulness, Self-Concept Clarity and Psychological Well-Being.” Personality
and Individual Differences 106:334–339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.10.028.

Hautakangas, M., K. Kumpulainen, and L. Uusitalo. 2021. “Children Developing Self-Regulation Skills in a
Kids’ Skills Intervention Programme in Finnish Early Childhood Education and Care.” Early Child
Development and Care 192 (10): 1626–1642. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2021.1918125.

James, A., and A. James. 2008. Key Concepts in Childhood Studies. London: Sage.
James, A., C. Jenks, and A. Prout. 1998. Theorizing Childhood. Cambridge: Polity Press.

Jenks, C. 2005. Childhood. London: Routledge.

Jerome, L., and H. Starkey. 2022. “Developing Children’s Agency Within a Children’s Rights Education
Framework: 10 Propositions.” Education 3-13 50 (4): 439–451. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004279.2022.
2052233.

Killeen, P. R., S. H. Tillery, and F. Cabrera. 2024. “Agency.” The Journal of General Psychology 1–51. https://
doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2024.2433277.

Klein, S. B. 2012. “The Self and its Brain.” Social Cognition 30 (4): 474–518. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2012.
30.4.474.

Lansdown, G., and R. Karkara. 2006. “Children’s Right to Express Views and Have Them Taken Seriously.”
The Lancet 367 (9511): 690–692. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68268-8.

Lundy, L., C. Murray, K. Smith, and C. Ward. 2024. “Young Children’s Right to be Heard on the Quality of
Their Education: Addressing Potential Misunderstandings in the Context of Early Childhood Education.”
British Educational Research Journal 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1002/berj.3968.

Manyukhina, Y. 2022. “Children’s Agency in the National Curriculum for England: A Critical Discourse
Analysis.” Education 3-13 50 (4): 506–520. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004279.2022.2052232.

Manyukhina, Y., and D. Wyse. 2019. “Learner Agency and the Curriculum: A Critical Realist Perspective.” The
Curriculum Journal 30 (3): 223–243. https://doi.org/10.1080/09585176.2019.1599973.

Manyukhina, Y, and D Wyse. 2021. “Children’s Agency: What is it, and What Should Be Done?.” BERA
News 1 March 2021. https://www.bera.ac.uk/blog/childrens-agency-what-is-it-and-what-should-be-done.

Maybin, J. 2013. “Towards a Sociocultural Understanding of Children’s Voice.” Language and Education 27
(5): 383–397. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500782.2012.704048.

Moss, P., and P. Petrie. 2002. From Children’s Services to Children’s Spaces. London: Routledge.
OECD. 2019. OECD Future of Education and Skills 2030. OECD Learning Compass 2030: A Series of Concept
Notes. https://www.oecd.org/content/dam/oecd/en/about/projects/edu/education-2040/1-1-learning-
compass/OECD_Learning_Compass_2030_Concept_Note_Series.pdf.

OHCHR. 1989. United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child. https://www.ohchr.org/en/instruments-
mechanisms/instruments/convention-rights-child.

OHCHR. 2006. UNCRC. General Comment No.7 (2005). Implementing Child Rights in Early Childhood. CRC/
C/GC/7/Rev.1. https://www.refworld.org/legal/general/crc/2006/en/40994.

Oswell, D. 2013. The Agency of Children. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Qvortrup, J. 1994. “Introduction.” In Childhood Matters: Social Theory, Practice and Politics, edited by J.

Qvortrup, M. Bardy, G. Sgritta, and H. Wintersberger. Wien: Avebury.

Ryan, R. M., and E. L. Deci. 2020. “Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation from a Self-Determination Theory
Perspective: Definitions, Theory, Practices, and Future Directions.” Contemporary Educational
Psychology 61:101860. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101860.

Schatzki, T. R. 2025. “Agency.” Information and Organisation 35 (1): 100553. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infoandorg.2024.100553.

Sewell, W. H. 1992. “A Theory of Structure: Duality, Agency, and Transformation.” American Journal of
Sociology 98 (1): 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1086/229967. https://www.jstor.org/stable/2781191.

Sirkko, R., T. Kyrönlampi, and A. M. Puroila. 2019. “Children’s Agency: Opportunities and Constraints.”
International Journal of Early Childhood 51 (3): 283–300. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13158-019-00252-5.

Spiteri, J. 2024. Children’s Voices and Agency: Ways of Listening in Early Childhood Quantitative, Qualitative
and Mixed Methods Research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Stern, D. 1985. The Interpersonal World of the Infant. New York: Basic Books.

Sutterlüty, F., and E. K. M. Tisdall. 2019. “Agency, Autonomy and Self-Determination: Questioning key
Concepts of Childhood Studies.” Global Studies of Childhood 9 (3): 183–187. https://doi.org/10.1177/2043610619860992.

Thomson, P., and L. Maloy. 2022. The Benefits of Art, Craft and Design Education in Schools: A Rapid Evidence
Review. Nottingham: University of Nottingham. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.26341.47847.

United Nations (UN). 2024. The Sustainable Development Goals Report 2024. Paris: United Nations.

Wang, J., and K. C. Barrett. 2013. “Mastery Motivation and Self-Regulation During Early Childhood.” In
Handbook of Self-Regulatory Processes in Development: New Directions and International Perspectives, edited by K. C. Barrett, N. A. Fox, G. A. Morgan, D. J. Fidler, and L. A. Daunhauer, 337–380.
New York: Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203080719.ch15.

Wyness, M. 2014. Childhood. Cambridge: Polity Press.

Jane Murray
Centre for Education and Research, University of Northampton, Northampton, UK
Jane.Murray@northampton.ac.uk http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7000-0901

READ ONLINE (I'm not sure this URL will work):
https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/epdf/10.1080/09669760.2025.2495432?needAccess=true

OR SEE:
https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/09669760.2025.2495432?af=R
... AND CLICK ON
View PDF
... AND A NEW BROWSER TAB WILL OPEN WITH THE ARTICLE

  • (https site) https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/09669760.2025.2495432?af=R
    [@nonymouse] [Guardster] [Proxify] [Anonymisierungsdienst]

  • Follow ups:

    Post a response:

    Nickname:

    Password:

    Email (optional):
    Subject:


    Message:


    Link URL (optional):

    Link Title (optional):


    Add your sigpic?

    Here are Seven Rules for posting on this forum.

    1. Do not post erotica or overly-detailed sexual discussions.
    2. Do not request, offer, or post links to illegal material, including pictures.
    3. Don't annoy the cogs.
    4. Do not reveal identifying details about yourself or other posters.
    5. Do not advocate or counsel sex with minors.
    6. Do not post admissions of, or accuse others of, potentially illegal activities.
    7. Do not request meetings with posters who are under age 18.

    Posts made to BoyChat are subject to inclusion in the monthly BoyChat Digest. If you do not want your posts archived in the BC Digest, or want specific posts of yours removed after inclusion, please email The BC Digest.